Dogs In the News: Emotions, Compassion, and the Iditarod


Different emotions were elicited by surprising Mal with a jack-in-the-box causing Mal to wrinkle the top of his head into a frown, praising him that resulted in erect ears, reprimanding him that caused him to flatten his ears with downcast eyes, disgusting him that also caused his ears to flatten, and inducing fear and anger causing Mal’s ears to prick up and show the whites of the eyes.

How good are we really at reading dogs?

The results of this very interesting study are as follows: “Happiness was by far the easiest emotion to recognise with 88 per cent of the volunteers correctly identifying it. Anger was identified by 70 per cent of participants. About 45 per cent of volunteers spotted when Mal was frightened, while 37 per cent could identify the relatively subtle emotion of sadness. The canine expressions that were hardest for humans to identify were surprise and disgust. …  those [people] with minimal experience of dogs were better at identifying negative emotions.”

Concerning the results of this study, Beverley Cuddy, editor of Dogs Today, notes, ”I am not at all surprised science has finally accepted what we knew all along – dog and owner communicate perfectly well without words.”

I’m not so sure I agree that we “communicate perfectly well without words.” We are pretty good at it but when one considers the results of this study for the ability to read anger, fright, and sadness there was considerable room for error as there was for surprise and disgust. Of course, only one dog with a unique history of contact with humans was studied and only 50 humans partook in this project so perhaps future research will support that we are actually better at reading what dogs are feeling. 

What about doggie guilt?

The Daily Mail essay begins, “We all know that guilty look on the dog’s face when he swipes a sausage from the kitchen work top. But it seems that’s not the only emotion we can recognise in our canine friends.” This is slightly misleading. Actually, we’re not all that good at reading guilt. Consider the results of Dr. Alexandra Horowitz’s research on reading canine guilt that showed we’re not very good at knowing when dogs are feeling guilty for doing something they weren’t supposed to do. Once again we need more data concerning just how good we are at reading guilt but I want to emphasize once again that Dr. Horowitz’s research does ot show that dogs do not feel guilt (for more on this and on the emotional lives of dogs please see). 

Rescued Fozzie dog shares food with scrawny and smelly Lars, a hungry stray

Here’s a story, written by Kirsten Stade of Peaceful Dog blog, that’ll warm your heart. On a romp through his neighborhood, Kirsten’s foster dog, a mastiff-pit-something-mix named Fozzie Bear, met and befriended a stray white German shepherd Kirsten named Lars. After Fozzie disappeared from her yard Kirsten went out to find him. She writes, “I caught up to Fozzie in a neighbor’s backyard. Flattened into a playful crouch, grinning broadly, his tongue extending nearly to the ground, he lay not 5 feet from a tall, scrawny, off-white dog who gave off a potent smell and was also smiling and panting up a storm. Not knowing where this dog came from, I thought maybe he would follow me home so I could get a better look at him. Once I leashed up Fozzie, sure enough, the white dog followed along behind us and even came right into my front yard.” It seemed as if Fozzie felt compassion for Lars and they went on to become the best of friends. 

What I love about this story is not only the passion with which Kirsten writes about Fozzie and Lars but also how she notes, “Lars would need an adopter of unusual sensitivity and experience, and much as I wanted to place him, I was not going to place him in a home that might let him down the way he’d been let down before.”

This is a great “up” story when you need a boost.

The Iditarod and the death of Dorado, a “dropped dog”

Fellow Psychology Today writer and good friend Mark Derr recently posted an essay called “Iditarod Dog Death Raises Howls of Protest: Husky dropped from the race across Alaska suffocates in a snow drift”. During the last running of this event, a four-year old dog named Dorado died after he was left behind and tethered by a rookie musher. Dorado suffocated in a snow drift because weather precluded rescuers from getting to him (see also). Dogs left behind for a variety of reasons are called “dropped dogs”. 

Mark has watched the Iditarod in person, whereas I have not. One can forever debate the pros and cons of the Iditarod, but Dorado’s death made me think about the fate of dropped dogs and how much pain and suffering they might endure while waiting to be picked up. And, shouldn’t it be required that someone remain with them until they are rescued? 

One part of his essay caught my eye. Mark notes, “Dogs have died during the race—approximately 140 according to PETA’s tally, a small percentage of those who have competed since the first Iditarod in 1973. The causes have ranged from moose attack to accidents to a variety of medical ailments.” No dogs competing in the Iditarod are known to have died from 2009-2012. (A comment posted in response to my essay also is concerned with the treatment of dogs during training for the race and their fate after they’ve raced.)

Tags:
anger, behavioural processes, belgian shepherd, bloom, daily mail, disgust, dogs, ears, facial expressions, happiness, harry friedman, jack in the box, minimal experience, nbsp, negative emotions, police dog, sadness, subtle emotion, volunteers, walden university

  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • email
  • StumbleUpon
  • Delicious
  • Google Reader
  • LinkedIn
  • BlinkList
  • Digg
  • Google Bookmarks
  • HackerNews
  • Posterous
  • Reddit
  • Sphinn
  • Tumblr
  • Tumblr
  • Tumblr